Hot: audiofile quality custom made cables ON THE CHEAP!

Page 2 - Seeking answers? Join the AnandTech community: where nearly half-a-million members share solutions and discuss the latest tech.

mikeford

Diamond Member
Jan 27, 2001
5,671
160
106
The trouble with 99% of the audiophile crap is that people only hear a difference as long as they can see which cable is being used. Once you hide the identity of the cable or other nutty audiophile device people stop hearing differences or think they do still hear a difference and pick the plain old cables half the time.

OTOH really good sound is a wonderfull thing that few enjoy.
 

sxr7171

Diamond Member
Jun 21, 2002
5,079
40
91
^^^ No you don't need anything crazy but, you can usually notice improvements by swapping out cables. I don't believe in $1000 cables, but sometimes you need a good cable for a quality system. I keep my cable costs to under 5% of the total cost of my system, which is considered very low in audiophilia. I have seen system recommendations which have cables making up as high 25% of the system cost, I don't subscribe to that philosophy.
 

dr150

Diamond Member
Sep 18, 2003
6,570
24
81
This argument between both sides whether cables make a difference will NEVER end.

There are truths on both sides. On one hand, it's ridiculous to spend too much money on cables. It's snake oil when you're buying $1000 interconnects which cost $20 to make.

Do cables make a difference? Depends. When you compare the stuff that comes with the equipment with great connectors tied to a good geometry & metal...AND when you have resolving enough speakers and front end, then YESSSS you can hear a difference. On cheaper equipment it won't make a difference.

The difference is most blatant, at least to me, with power cords. When I placed good power cords on my amps, the bass deepened substantially. It was quite an amazing spectacle to see such a drastic effect.

...........For bang/buck good quality/design cable look at Blue Jeans Cables or Signal Cables....just google the names.
 

sxr7171

Diamond Member
Jun 21, 2002
5,079
40
91
^^^ Yeah, I'm going to do power cords next (if my next upgrade isn't a new turntable). I had a dealer try to sell me the Transparent Plus Powerlink for around $230, but I decided to wait until I read about it being the best in that price range. He told me to take it home and try it, and if it didn't make a difference to send it back. I wish that I did buy them there just to try out over the weekend (of course I would have kept them if they made a significant difference), but I'd have to ship it back if I didn't like it (the dealer was about in town about 2 hours away and I was just visiting town).

Which power cables have worked well for you?
 

Nebor

Lifer
Jun 24, 2003
29,582
12
76
Originally posted by: Cybordolphin
This is interconnect cable not speaker cable.

If you don't realize the difference a high quality interconnect can make. Come over.

Are you coming onto me?
 

S13SilviaK

Senior member
Jan 23, 2002
991
0
71
Taken from a 'friend'...Double-blind test is the only proof, and anybody who says they can hear a difference between a $20 cable and a $20000 cable hasn't done a double-blind.
 

ValsalvaYourHeartOut

Senior member
Apr 30, 2001
777
0
0
BWAHAHAHAH. Those of you who believe expensive interconnects make any audible difference are big SUCKERS...(probably members of that Raptor thread.) But I have to admit that the high-end stuff looks really cool...that's about it.

If you want some data, here's a good place to startLink
If anyone has any links to well-conducted double-blind controlled experiments that demonstrate that there IS a difference, pelase post. thanks.

Valsalva
 

Sanjoelo

Senior member
Apr 4, 2001
809
0
0
"Audiophiles" are a scary lot. Anything that ends in "phile" usually isn't complimentary.

I attended an audio club meeting locally, once. I like great quality sound, but to sit around a room and claim you can hear a "slight change in the subtlety of a symbol crash" is about as stimulating as watching a culture grow green fuzz. Most of these guys were in their forties and fifties and still had Mom's home cookin' waiting on the table when they got home, if you get my gist.

I mean, c'mon--once you've achieved great sound from a good quality audio system, who gives a rat's arse about barely detectable subtleties? Do good interconnects make a difference? Sure, to a degree. But to spend hundreds or thousands on them is just a pacifier for an otherwise empty life. And man, life's too short to be trying to detect pitch changes in a triangle tap. It's just bragging rights, but you may as well piss in the wind, 'cause nobody cares about your XTK-7000MC Master Series Proton Custom UltraMega Neutralized Super Mawamba Mojo Interconnect. Except other "'philes", that is. And they forgot about it as soon as they got home and realized the new Hustler came in the mail.

Musically, nothing still compares to a live performance anyway. Do I believe in owning nice audio equipment? Sure. Good interconnects? Yep. To spend thousands on them? Get real. Even if it DOES make a slight difference--who cares? Get out once in a while. Sheesh.

I get a kick of of sites like bettercables.com. Give me a break.





 

ValsalvaYourHeartOut

Senior member
Apr 30, 2001
777
0
0
Originally posted by: dr150
This argument between both sides whether cables make a difference will NEVER end.

There are truths on both sides. On one hand, it's ridiculous to spend too much money on cables. It's snake oil when you're buying $1000 interconnects which cost $20 to make.

Do cables make a difference? Depends. When you compare the stuff that comes with the equipment with great connectors tied to a good geometry & metal...AND when you have resolving enough speakers and front end, then YESSSS you can hear a difference. On cheaper equipment it won't make a difference.

The difference is most blatant, at least to me, with power cords. When I placed good power cords on my amps, the bass deepened substantially. It was quite an amazing spectacle to see such a drastic effect.

...........For bang/buck good quality/design cable look at Blue Jeans Cables or Signal Cables....just google the names.

You have got to he kidding me...better bass with the POWER CORDS?????? Uh, did it ever occur to you that you have hundreds of feet of household wiring (unshielded, small gauge, subject to noise) prior to your upgraded power cord? Absolutely hilarious.

Look, people. Regardless of how high-end your equipment is, your ultra-expensive interconnects and cables make absolutely no difference at all...read: INAUDIBLE differences.

The dramatic differences people report are ALWAYS done non-blinded - which means the listener knows what cable he is reviewing, and the placebo effect kicks in. That is, even if there are absolutely no differences, the person's preconceived notions will influence his perception of the cable. It's a very well-described phenomenon. You can take a group of audiophiles and ask them to listen to cheap radio shack cable...then tell them you're swapping in some $100/foot stuff and ask them what they hear...every single one of them will tell you "wow, the soundstage improved dramatically, the bass was tighter, amazing!!"...when in reality, you never really switched the cables. Cables are the biggest scam that ever existed, and the lies are perpetuated by the profitability of selling these products.

Time and time again, whenever people conduct double-blind comparisons (in which the listener doesn't know which cable he's listening to), nobody is able to pick out the expensive vs. cheap cable more often that we'd expect by chance (i.e. random guessing).

The problem is, people out there have already convinced themselves that expensive cables make an audible difference (and have spent hundreds/thousands of dollars already) - these are the people who can never accept the truth. But as long as people "think" they have better sound quality (whether or not it's really there), I guess it's okay. Not MY money.

Valsalva
 

mikeford

Diamond Member
Jan 27, 2001
5,671
160
106
$230 for a power cable? Does this dealer give you a cookie after you get your lovin?
 

LegendKiller

Lifer
Mar 5, 2001
18,256
68
86
Have YOU people ever listened to a good audio setup that has crappy cables and then switched to good ones?


I, for one, have seen what crap cables can do to a system. I had a cheap Rat shack "subwoofer" cable that I got for a decent price. I didn't use it since I did not have a sub. HOwever, when I got my HSU VTF-2, I stuck it on there. The thing hummed so loud that I had to unplug it right away.

I talked to a few people and was advised to get a nice cable from www.bluejeanscable.com. They make cables out of nice Belden and Canare connectors. These aren't hugely expensive, but are definitely much better quality than RS or Monster. The humming went away.

You people can sit here and say this crap over and over. However, you really have no f'ing clue what your talking about. Even if you THINK you know what a good setup is and what "good" cables are, you STILL have no idea. You haven't ever owned what these people have owned and you have never seen what htye have seen. I have heard nice setups and I have heard crap ones that are equally expensive. However, I bet most of the nay-sayers on here have nothing more than a trash sony system that puts out "100 watts per channel".

A well balanced system will ALWAYS sound good. This means a good receiver, a good audio source, and cables that are nice to match up.

I have NEVER seen ONE test that any scientist would call valid. The garbage that you people spout is usually full of horrible errors that ruin the results for a logical person. I went through 7 years of education to set up a proper experiment, and what you have provided me so far isn't anything close. You need a large sample base, repeatable experiments in the same conditions, all the same variables, and most importantly, a good research staff.


What this basically comes down to, is there are haves and have-nots. The have-nots are always going to look and say "Wow, these people are stupid for wasting money". The haves are going to say "Wow, these people are stupid for wasting good money on crap stuff". Why not just shut up and let everybody buy their own stuff?
 

sxr7171

Diamond Member
Jun 21, 2002
5,079
40
91
Originally posted by: mikeford
$230 for a power cable? Does this dealer give you a cookie after you get your lovin?



When you have a $20,000 system $230 isn't a lot of money for a cable. I wouldn't have kept them if they didn't make a good difference either. I have no problem running dedicated wiring from the fuse box to the system via decent power wire from home depot and a good hospital grade outlet ($15) if I need to.

I can't say if it would have made a big difference without trying it but I have heard what getting a good set of speaker cable has done for my system, and getting a good budget set of interconnects has done. Eventually I'm going to try an aftermarket power cable and if it makes a good difference I will keep it. If the cable happens to be a $50 quality budget cord and I see no need to spend more then I won't spend more.
 

ValsalvaYourHeartOut

Senior member
Apr 30, 2001
777
0
0
Originally posted by: LegendKiller
Have YOU people ever listened to a good audio setup that has crappy cables and then switched to good ones?

Yep...and single-blinded with a friend helping me, I could not tell the difference... dramatic differences do not exist when using blinded comparisons.

I, for one, have seen what crap cables can do to a system. I had a cheap Rat shack "subwoofer" cable that I got for a decent price. I didn't use it since I did not have a sub. HOwever, when I got my HSU VTF-2, I stuck it on there. The thing hummed so loud that I had to unplug it right away.

If it hummed, there was probably some defect in it or your ran the cable near a noise source. There's no way that any intact shielded cable will cause that kind of hum - and you don't need to be an audiophile to figure that one out. And even if your bluejean cables had superior shielding, there's no way in hell you can have several orders of magnitude of improved noise rejection to result in audible hum to nothing...unless you have an active filter (which you don't). An incorrect conclusion in your case would be to attribute the hum to "cheap cables"...a more correct analysis would point to other things.

And BTW, I have a Hsu TN-1220HO and it made absolutely no difference when I switched from 12 ga home depot wire to the equivalent of 8 ga 100% OFC flat cable...none.

I talked to a few people and was advised to get a nice cable from www.bluejeanscable.com. They make cables out of nice Belden and Canare connectors. These aren't hugely expensive, but are definitely much better quality than RS or Monster. The humming went away.

Yeah, that's it....cheap cables have hum. That's almost as delusional as the guy who thought upgrading his power cable dramatically improved his bass.

You people can sit here and say this crap over and over. However, you really have no f'ing clue what your talking about. Even if you THINK you know what a good setup is and what "good" cables are, you STILL have no idea. You haven't ever owned what these people have owned and you have never seen what htye have seen. I have heard nice setups and I have heard crap ones that are equally expensive. However, I bet most of the nay-sayers on here have nothing more than a trash sony system that puts out "100 watts per channel".

Actually, I have a pretty decent B&W, Hsu, and NAD setup, and I also have a pair of Senn HD-580's. If high-end cables and interconnects produce the dramatic sound improvements promised, I should have no problem differentiating. Unfortunately, the measurable resistance, capacitance, and inductance of low-end and high-end wires/cables are virtually the same, and all within the realm of inaudibility. You can't argue with physics, nor can you argue with the fact that double-blinded comparisons always fail to demonstrate audible differences (when level matched).

A well balanced system will ALWAYS sound good. This means a good receiver, a good audio source, and cables that are nice to match up.

This is what they tell you at the high-end audio store to get you to buy expensive stuff... you can't always believe everything you hear from the saleguy.

I have NEVER seen ONE test that any scientist would call valid. The garbage that you people spout is usually full of horrible errors that ruin the results for a logical person. I went through 7 years of education to set up a proper experiment, and what you have provided me so far isn't anything close. You need a large sample base, repeatable experiments in the same conditions, all the same variables, and most importantly, a good research staff.

I disagree...you don't need a double-blinded multi-center trial published in a top peer-reviewed journal in order to arrive at a reasonable conclusion that cables don't make a difference. The problem is that cable manufacturers claim DRAMATIC improvements in sound quality. DRAMATIC. So a simple blinded study involving identical equipment, level adjustment, and a few trusted listeners should be able to allow people to EASILY differentiate a high-end cable from zip-cord based on these DRAMATIC differences. Unfortunately, these experiments always lead to the same conclusion: listeners are not able to identify the expensive cable at an accuracy level greater than that expected by chance (guessing). If these cable differences are as profound and audible as is claimed by the manufacturer, then WHY OH WHY can't we hear these differences in a controlled comparison? Simple: there ARE NO DIFFERENCES.

The only time you hear reports of cable differences is when there is NO BLINDING. This means a listener knows which cable he is hearing, and he THINKS he hears differences. It's the placebo effect, or the influence of preconceived notions on one's perceptions....a very well-described phenomenon.

What this basically comes down to, is there are haves and have-nots. The have-nots are always going to look and say "Wow, these people are stupid for wasting money". The haves are going to say "Wow, these people are stupid for wasting good money on crap stuff". Why not just shut up and let everybody buy their own stuff?

I actually bi-wire my speakers with Monster Cable, rather than zip cord...why? Because I like the way it looks...and it feels better knowing that I'm using expensive cable to power my setup. But I don't for a minute believe that it makes ANY difference in sound quality...and if I want to spend my money that way, then fine.
But the problem is that there are people who may be MISLED into purchasing expensive cable because they THINK it'll improve their sound quality. In reality, it won't, and it'll be a waste of their money....like buying snake oil. So why don't YOU shut up and stop spreading misinformation when you clearly have been brainwashed by the high-end cable industry.

Valsalva
 

sxr7171

Diamond Member
Jun 21, 2002
5,079
40
91
Originally posted by: ValsalvaYourHeartOut
Originally posted by: LegendKiller
Have YOU people ever listened to a good audio setup that has crappy cables and then switched to good ones?

Yep...and single-blinded with a friend helping me, I could not tell the difference... dramatic differences do not exist when using blinded comparisons.

I, for one, have seen what crap cables can do to a system. I had a cheap Rat shack "subwoofer" cable that I got for a decent price. I didn't use it since I did not have a sub. HOwever, when I got my HSU VTF-2, I stuck it on there. The thing hummed so loud that I had to unplug it right away.

If it hummed, there was probably some defect in it or your ran the cable near a noise source. There's no way that any intact shielded cable will cause that kind of hum - and you don't need to be an audiophile to figure that one out. And even if your bluejean cables had superior shielding, there's no way in hell you can have several orders of magnitude of improved noise rejection to result in audible hum to nothing...unless you have an active filter (which you don't). An incorrect conclusion in your case would be to attribute the hum to "cheap cables"...a more correct analysis would point to other things.

And BTW, I have a Hsu TN-1220HO and it made absolutely no difference when I switched from 12 ga home depot wire to the equivalent of 8 ga 100% OFC flat cable...none.

I talked to a few people and was advised to get a nice cable from www.bluejeanscable.com. They make cables out of nice Belden and Canare connectors. These aren't hugely expensive, but are definitely much better quality than RS or Monster. The humming went away.

Yeah, that's it....cheap cables have hum. That's almost as delusional as the guy who thought upgrading his power cable dramatically improved his bass.

You people can sit here and say this crap over and over. However, you really have no f'ing clue what your talking about. Even if you THINK you know what a good setup is and what "good" cables are, you STILL have no idea. You haven't ever owned what these people have owned and you have never seen what htye have seen. I have heard nice setups and I have heard crap ones that are equally expensive. However, I bet most of the nay-sayers on here have nothing more than a trash sony system that puts out "100 watts per channel".

Actually, I have a pretty decent B&W, Hsu, and NAD setup, and I also have a pair of Senn HD-580's. If high-end cables and interconnects produce the dramatic sound improvements promised, I should have no problem differentiating. Unfortunately, the measurable resistance, capacitance, and inductance of low-end and high-end wires/cables are virtually the same, and all within the realm of inaudibility. You can't argue with physics, nor can you argue with the fact that double-blinded comparisons always fail to demonstrate audible differences (when level matched).

A well balanced system will ALWAYS sound good. This means a good receiver, a good audio source, and cables that are nice to match up.

This is what they tell you at the high-end audio store to get you to buy expensive stuff... you can't always believe everything you hear from the saleguy.

I have NEVER seen ONE test that any scientist would call valid. The garbage that you people spout is usually full of horrible errors that ruin the results for a logical person. I went through 7 years of education to set up a proper experiment, and what you have provided me so far isn't anything close. You need a large sample base, repeatable experiments in the same conditions, all the same variables, and most importantly, a good research staff.

I disagree...you don't need a double-blinded multi-center trial published in a top peer-reviewed journal in order to arrive at a reasonable conclusion that cables don't make a difference. The problem is that cable manufacturers claim DRAMATIC improvements in sound quality. DRAMATIC. So a simple blinded study involving identical equipment, level adjustment, and a few trusted listeners should be able to allow people to EASILY differentiate a high-end cable from zip-cord based on these DRAMATIC differences. Unfortunately, these experiments always lead to the same conclusion: listeners are not able to identify the expensive cable at an accuracy level greater than that expected by chance (guessing). If these cable differences are as profound and audible as is claimed by the manufacturer, then WHY OH WHY can't we hear these differences in a controlled comparison? Simple: there ARE NO DIFFERENCES.

The only time you hear reports of cable differences is when there is NO BLINDING. This means a listener knows which cable he is hearing, and he THINKS he hears differences. It's the placebo effect, or the influence of preconceived notions on one's perceptions....a very well-described phenomenon.

What this basically comes down to, is there are haves and have-nots. The have-nots are always going to look and say "Wow, these people are stupid for wasting money". The haves are going to say "Wow, these people are stupid for wasting good money on crap stuff". Why not just shut up and let everybody buy their own stuff?

I actually bi-wire my speakers with Monster Cable, rather than zip cord...why? Because I like the way it looks...and it feels better knowing that I'm using expensive cable to power my setup. But I don't for a minute believe that it makes ANY difference in sound quality...and if I want to spend my money that way, then fine.
But the problem is that there are people who may be MISLED into purchasing expensive cable because they THINK it'll improve their sound quality. In reality, it won't, and it'll be a waste of their money....like buying snake oil. So why don't YOU shut up and stop spreading misinformation when you clearly have been brainwashed by the high-end cable industry.

Valsalva



Could you hear a difference even when you were shown the cable being used? Maybe you simply cannot hear the difference between cables blind or not blind. BTW, judging a cable by how it affects a sub isn't a good way to judge a cable. I used to use the cheapest cable to run to my sub back in the days when I used a sub in my system. Cables are best judged by how they affect the mid-bass up. The main reson for this is that the human ear isn't even very good at resolving low bass.

If you can't hear a difference between cables then more power to you, but your words won't change what I heard when I first heard the difference a good speaker made in my system or the difference that spending an extra $60 to trade up to a used pair of decent interconnects made to my system. If you can't hear it, then accpet that you can't hear it. Of course it requires a little bit of open mindedness to actually listen for yourself instead of quoting a couple of ABX studies.
 

dr150

Diamond Member
Sep 18, 2003
6,570
24
81
Originally posted by: sxr7171
^^^ Yeah, I'm going to do power cords next (if my next upgrade isn't a new turntable). I had a dealer try to sell me the Transparent Plus Powerlink for around $230, but I decided to wait until I read about it being the best in that price range. He told me to take it home and try it, and if it didn't make a difference to send it back. I wish that I did buy them there just to try out over the weekend (of course I would have kept them if they made a significant difference), but I'd have to ship it back if I didn't like it (the dealer was about in town about 2 hours away and I was just visiting town).

Which power cables have worked well for you?

Signal Cables offer the best bang for buck in audiophile power cords. Really great stuff! :D
 

ValsalvaYourHeartOut

Senior member
Apr 30, 2001
777
0
0
Originally posted by: sxr7171Could you hear a difference even when you were shown the cable being used? Maybe you simply cannot hear the difference between cables blind or not blind.

Maybe I cannot hear a difference between cables because there ARE no differences to hear.

BTW, judging a cable by how it affects a sub isn't a good way to judge a cable. I used to use the cheapest cable to run to my sub back in the days when I used a sub in my system. Cables are best judged by how they affect the mid-bass up. The main reson for this is that the human ear isn't even very good at resolving low bass.

Hey, when cable companies describe dramatic improvements in bass clarity (blah blah), I'd expect to hear a difference. I don't. There aren't any to hear. If the "human ear isn't very good at resolving low bass," then why even spend money on a decent sub? I guarantee that I can hear and FEEL down to 20Hz, and there are definitely differences among the sub's I've listened to...unfortunately, I can't say the same for the cables used to hook them up.

If you can't hear a difference between cables then more power to you, but your words won't change what I heard when I first heard the difference a good speaker made in my system or the difference that spending an extra $60 to trade up to a used pair of decent interconnects made to my system. If you can't hear it, then accpet that you can't hear it. Of course it requires a little bit of open mindedness to actually listen for yourself instead of quoting a couple of ABX studies.

Oh boy, I can easily tell when there's a difference in sound quality...and I can assure you that there wasn't any to be heard. This isn't a matter of "golden ears"...it's not like I'm can't hear the difference between a Bose and a pair of electrostatics. I've compared Monster Cable to the cheap stuff, and I can assure you it makes no difference...if the improvements were as dramatic as they claim, I should have absolutely no problem hearing them...but I don't...cuz there are no differences.

People have done measurements of the resistance, capacitance, and inductance of high-end cables vs. cheap zip-cord...no differences sufficient to explain differences in sound quality. People have done blind AB comparisons between cables and I have yet to see ONE study that shows a single person able to differentiate between expensive and cheap cables.

What you thought you heard is probably the product of the placebo effect....when people expect to hear dramatic differences when they upgrade their cables, they will actually perceive such improvements, regardless of whether or not there really is a difference. The only way to control for the placebo effect is to BLIND your listener. You can argue that the listeners chosen for the blinded AB comparisons are not capable of hearing the subtle differences that exist...this point is always brought up in the debate. The thing people forget is the people selected as listeners are not deaf - they are generally trained audiophiles with extensive experience in listening to high-end systems...and as I pointed out before, if cables truly made a HUGE difference in sound, it should be ABSOLUTELY NO CHALLENGE for these listeners to pick out the expensive cables from the cheap ones...but time and time again, they are not able to.

With no plausible electrical explanation for difference in sound quality (e.g. equivalent resistance/capacitance/inductance) and data that also fails to demonstrate differences in sound quality, any reasonable person would conclude that indeed, high-end cables do NOT improve your sound. Unfortunately, the myth of expensive cables/interconnects has been perpetuated and imprinted into everyone's head that most people think you're crazy if you tell them otherwise...even in the face of overwhelming data. People "choose" to believe that these cables will help their stereo systems...they want to believe it...it's so comforting to imagine the signal from your $2000 Krell amp is travelling through some exotic $200 cable. Too bad the cables don't do squat.

Valsalva
 

sxr7171

Diamond Member
Jun 21, 2002
5,079
40
91
Originally posted by: ValsalvaYourHeartOut
Originally posted by: sxr7171Could you hear a difference even when you were shown the cable being used? Maybe you simply cannot hear the difference between cables blind or not blind.

Maybe I cannot hear a difference between cables because there ARE no differences to hear.

BTW, judging a cable by how it affects a sub isn't a good way to judge a cable. I used to use the cheapest cable to run to my sub back in the days when I used a sub in my system. Cables are best judged by how they affect the mid-bass up. The main reson for this is that the human ear isn't even very good at resolving low bass.

Hey, when cable companies describe dramatic improvements in bass clarity (blah blah), I'd expect to hear a difference. I don't. There aren't any to hear. If the "human ear isn't very good at resolving low bass," then why even spend money on a decent sub? I guarantee that I can hear and FEEL down to 20Hz, and there are definitely differences among the sub's I've listened to...unfortunately, I can't say the same for the cables used to hook them up.

If you can't hear a difference between cables then more power to you, but your words won't change what I heard when I first heard the difference a good speaker made in my system or the difference that spending an extra $60 to trade up to a used pair of decent interconnects made to my system. If you can't hear it, then accpet that you can't hear it. Of course it requires a little bit of open mindedness to actually listen for yourself instead of quoting a couple of ABX studies.

Oh boy, I can easily tell when there's a difference in sound quality...and I can assure you that there wasn't any to be heard. This isn't a matter of "golden ears"...it's not like I'm can't hear the difference between a Bose and a pair of electrostatics. I've compared Monster Cable to the cheap stuff, and I can assure you it makes no difference...if the improvements were as dramatic as they claim, I should have absolutely no problem hearing them...but I don't...cuz there are no differences.

People have done measurements of the resistance, capacitance, and inductance of high-end cables vs. cheap zip-cord...no differences sufficient to explain differences in sound quality. People have done blind AB comparisons between cables and I have yet to see ONE study that shows a single person able to differentiate between expensive and cheap cables.

What you thought you heard is probably the product of the placebo effect....when people expect to hear dramatic differences when they upgrade their cables, they will actually perceive such improvements, regardless of whether or not there really is a difference. The only way to control for the placebo effect is to BLIND your listener. You can argue that the listeners chosen for the blinded AB comparisons are not capable of hearing the subtle differences that exist...this point is always brought up in the debate. The thing people forget is the people selected as listeners are not deaf - they are generally trained audiophiles with extensive experience in listening to high-end systems...and as I pointed out before, if cables truly made a HUGE difference in sound, it should be ABSOLUTELY NO CHALLENGE for these listeners to pick out the expensive cables from the cheap ones...but time and time again, they are not able to.

With no plausible electrical explanation for difference in sound quality (e.g. equivalent resistance/capacitance/inductance) and data that also fails to demonstrate differences in sound quality, any reasonable person would conclude that indeed, high-end cables do NOT improve your sound. Unfortunately, the myth of expensive cables/interconnects has been perpetuated and imprinted into everyone's head that most people think you're crazy if you tell them otherwise...even in the face of overwhelming data. People "choose" to believe that these cables will help their stereo systems...they want to believe it...it's so comforting to imagine the signal from your $2000 Krell amp is travelling through some exotic $200 cable. Too bad the cables don't do squat.

Valsalva


There are differences between subs and it depends mostly on a sub being able to control distortion - mainly upper harmonics. Other factors are the usual such as bass extension, ability to handle transients, good dynamics etc. Bass clarity is actually carried by lower midrange frequencies or even higher. The audibility of bass is actually present in the accurate harmonics of the bass signal. Localization of bass is in due to harmonics as well (which I'm sure you already know or you wouldn't have a sub). So it is the midrange driver that handles such matters as clarity and character of bass (this is all asuming that the sub is of good enough quality not to add it's own distortions and harmonics). This is why I don't believe that spending money on a subwoofer cable is very important.

We don't even know which cables you tested to come to your conclusion. I have heard for myself the difference a good cable can make and I believe that in the least cable geometry makes a difference in sound, because I heard the difference immediately. It didn't take 5 minutes to hear it. I'll admit that when I went from lamp cord to a $50 pair of Kimbers it didn't make a difference at all, but there were other factors involved.

I had an NAD 314 powering a pair of PSB Alphas and at times I'd switch to my NHT Super Ones depending on what I felt like using. The whole setup was on a shelf in a dorm room. When I think back to those days I'd have to say that my system sounded like crap in that room. Then I got an apartment and got good stands for the speakers, I didn't really compare cables at that point (lamp cord would have been my only comparison) but I still don't think those Kimbers were very good.

About a year later I ordered my new speakers with a pair of Analysis Plus cables (oval 12 biwire) that the manufacturer recommended (he gave me a good discount and he uses the same cabling for internal wiring in the speaker). The cables came two days before the speakers so I hooked them up to my NAD and the Super Ones in the meantime. I was honestly skeptical about these cables, I just bought them because they weren't ridiculously expensive and would be fitting of my new (significantly more expensive) speakers. It didn't take me even a couple of seconds to hear the (surprisingly large) difference, and I didn't even care to hear a difference either. I just assumed that my NAD/NHT stuff was too low end for cables to make a difference. It actually made my system a lot more listenable and I really enjoyed it for the next two days.

I also switched recently from the cheapest Audioquest interconnects to a $65 used cable from Australia, the difference wasn't as significant as my experience with my speaker cables but definitely tangible.

Considering the value of my system the amount I have spent on cable is a piddling amount since I still want value for my money, however I have heard the difference and I won't forget it. The whole experience has made me a lot more open minded about things we claim to fully understand but we really don't.

BTW, most ABX studies were done decades ago with a whole different class of equipment and conditions. The studies you linked to often had only one listener in the sample group. I have no doubt that if someone tried to DBT me with the Analysis Plus cable and the Kimber I had, I would easily show that I could distinguish between the two.


For more information read below (all from www.audioasylum.com and Jon Risch):



See:
http://www.audioasylum.com/forums/prophead/messages/2190.html
and at:
http://www.audioasylum.com/forums/prophead/messages/2579.html
and
http://www.audioasylum.com/forums/prophead/messages/2580.html

Please read them all the way through, or you wll be making comments that are not relevant. I feel that instantaneuos switching, with it's attendant extra wiring and contacts, is basically one way to invalidate such a test. I outline those reasons quite thoroughly in the referenced posts.

Some folks refuse to do a proper DBT, which require a lot of work and effort ot do correctly, and a half-assed DBT is worse than no DBT at all, again for reasons outlined in the referenced posts.
For these gfolks, I would suggest long trm sighted listening, swapping in the cable that is reputed to be the better one and listening to this one for several weeks straight, and THEN swapping in the old or "inferior" cable later. This would be especially important with the two cables you propose. It would also be necessary to assure tha trhe interconnet was nto a limiting factor, so that it did not reduce everything to a lowest common denominator (in other words, become a limiting quality factor).


Jon Risch


DBTs, ABX and the Meaning of Life? Part 1

Talking about Double Blind Tests is worse than discussing politics or religion, and the infamous DBT thread death-spiral is all too familiar to most of us who have perused the various audio message boards or news groups on the Internet.

I personally have been accused of being anti-DBT, because I moderate a message board forum, the Cable Asylum, that has an anti-DBT posting rule. Nothing could be further from the truth.

I have often come out against the unwarranted conclusions that a few certain folks come to over the results of a few certain amateur listening tests, primarily because I am more familiar than most (but not more than jj) with the problems and limitations of such listening tests.

So I am going to discuss some of the various issues and aspects of DBT's, and in this, I am primarily referring to the amateur type of listening tests, not the professionally conducted types that jj and the codec folks do. I am not going to place this disclaimer at the end of every paragraph, so jj, please print out that sentence, and attach manually. If I specifically refer to a professionally conducted test, I will say so, quite clearly.

I also may refer to audio cable testing at times, but really, what I am saying applies to almost all audio component testing, and is relevant and applicable.

Now that that is out of the way, let's get down to brass tacks.
What is valid? That is, what is a valid listening test, or what constitutes a scientifically valid set of data?

The gold standard for many years has been serious studies or papers published in a peer-reviewed professional journal. There are many reasons for this, and I am not going to cover all of them. Suffice it to say, that this kind of presentation allows one to examine all the facts, the procedures, and the data. It provides for the review of the paper and it's contents by peers in the field, and is published where other professionals have access to it and can question it or raise points they feel have been overlooked. Does such publication guarantee that the conclusions reached by the author are pretty solid evidence? No, but it does provide a certain minimal level of information, screening and review that make the data and conclusions useful to a certain point.

DBT test results from certain amateur listening tests get thrown about sometimes as if they were some sort of hard, cold facts; after all, it was "scientifically determined" that such and such was the case, right?

However, when we look at these DBT listening tests more closely, we find that most have not been published in a peer-reviewed professional journal, in fact, not one DBT on audio cables has been published in such a manner. None. Very few listening tests on other audio components, with the exception of codecs, have been so published either. There have been a few landmark studies on speakers, ala Toole, and most people agree that audio loudspeaker systems do sound different, so this is not one of the more controversial components of study.

So why all the noise about DBT's? Where have they been published, and are they valid evidence? Well, for audio cables, only a handful have been published in popular press magazines. Note that this is not the same thing as being published in a professional journal, an editor may or may not have an agenda, no one else may be reviewing the article for accuracy or proper scientific procedures, etc. When I say just a handful, this is literally the case, as there are only about a half dozen (depends on your criteria) on speaker cables, and few on interconnects. For other audio components, there may be a half dozen articles or so. Not all of these came up with null results either, so it would be very hard to come to any sort of real conclusion based on the data from these articles.

What about web sites, message board posts, news group posts? These are what is known as anecdotal data, they usually have not provided all the details of the tests, nor all of the data, nor have they been reviewed by anyone for proper scientific procedures, etc.

The vast majority of listening test accounts are of an anecdotal nature, and not traditionally allowed to be considered as any sort of good scientifically based evidence.

So the very thing that is being argued about, the DBT listening tests, on audio components, are not of a nature that one can say are very useful in terms of truly valid scientific evidence.

So what about these amateur listening tests, these anecdotal web sites, the popular press magazine articles, are they any good to make any judgments from?

One of the great little catch phrases that get used by some folks extolling these amateur DBTs, is that "In 20/25/30 years of testing, no one has found XXX audio component to sound different, under controled conditions, when nothing is broken", etc.

This is meant to sound like DBT tests have put the matter to bed years ago. This sounds all very well and fine, until you realize: what was the SOTA for 25 or 30 years ago? What kind of cables would have been compared 25 or 30 years ago? I can tell you: zip cords against zip cords. Several of the articles commonly referred to by folks citing popular press DBT results, were this old. Some of the articles on CDPs are 17, 14 years old. How far have CDPs come in that length of time? I mean, we are talking about CDPs that probably did not even have 16 or 15 bits of resolution, no dither, multi-stage, multi-opamp analog output flters, etc.

So if you stop to think about how valid, how relevant some of these really old tests are to the current state of audio, including mid-fi, then it becomes clear that some of them are not really of any use for modern audio components.

What about the tests themselves, how were they conducted? Let's look at a typical scenario for one of the more popular testing paradigms of the day: an ABX style listening test. Note that this is not intended to represent ALL such tests, but merely to provide some idea of what went on in many of the amateur listening tests commonly cited.

First, an ABX switchbox was used to connect the two DUT's, or Device Under Test. In most cases, this required additional cables to be used to insert the switchbox into the signal chain, so it could control which unit was being heard at any given time. The extra cables were almost without exception, just zip cords and/or el cheapo IC's. Even when an audio cable was the subject of the test, the extra cable portion was almost always a zip cord or an el cheapo IC. The reasoning here was that both units were subjected to the same conditions, so it shouldn't matter. So much for the weakest link.

For cable tests, this would be a serious limiting factor, as whatever losses or problems the zip cords or cheap ICs had, were now superimposed on the test cables as well. Ironically, since the vast majority of testers did not believe that audio cables had any sonic impact, they created a situation that virtually guaranteed that it would be hard, at best, to hear what was going on.

Then the listener is asked to listen to the test units, and 'familiarize' themselves with the switchbox and listening protocol.
Typically, while the listener was listening, and switching back and forth, the music was allowed to play on. The first portion was the so-called sighted portion of the test, where they knew the identity of each unit (they know which one is A, and which one is B). The listener was often encouraged to switch back and forth during this portion, and to state whether or not they felt they were hearing the same kinds of sonic differences they did under sighted listening without the switchbox. More on this aspect later.

Then after what might have been hundreds of switches back and forth, under what I would call fairly casual conditions, they would enter the forced choice portion of the listening test, and be asked to identify an unknown DUT, presented as X. They still had access to hearing DUT A or B, and still knew what device the A or B unit was, but X was an unknown, and they were asked to make a choice as to whether it was unit A or unit B.

Classically, they were exposed to a total of 16 trials where they had to select what unit they thought X was, and since it was what is known as a forced choice type of situation, even if they were to readily admit, that they did not think they could identify the DUT, or that they had listening fatigue, they still were supposed to make a choice.
Note that each trial could consist of as many switches back and forth from A to B and back again, and to X and back again.

A single listener might only participate in a single run of 16 trials, and there might only be a handful of such listeners.

Once the listening tests were completed, then the test administrator would check the ABX hardware for the accuracy scores of the listener, and check this against a table of probability ratings, to see how much of a probability existed that the listener had actually been identifying the DUT beyond a certain level of sheer chance.

The benchmark for the 16 trials was to get 12 or more correct, this would then establish that the listener had less than a 5% chance of just guessing that many correct. It is what is known as a confidence level of 95%. The criteria for what was considered 'good enough' so as to not be just due to chance, is supposed to be selected before the test, and then adhered to. Other confidence levels could be used, such as 99% (very strict, and usually extremely hard to do in these kinds of tests), or 90%. It should be noted, that for a 95% confidence level, that just conducting 20 runs would typically result in one that appeared to exceeded the 95% confidence level, even if everything was just random choices. So in order to take the test results as a valid positive, one would have to do better than this on the average.

Much was made of these kinds of tests, mainly because of the fact that they were Double Blind, due to the use of the automated switchbox hardware. The test administrator did not know the identity of the X unit until after the test was completed, and therefore, was theoretically incapable of influencing the outcome of the tests.

What were the problems with these early amateur DBTs?

Unfortunately, they were legion.

It was often assumed that since these tests were double blind, that they represented the only 'true' kind of valid listening test available. However, it was often overlooked that the mere fact that any given listening test was DBT, did not guarantee ANYTHING else at all. It could have been the worlds worst listening test ever, and still could have been double blind.

The long open (sighted) initial portion was not really training, nor were they valid controls of the test sensitivity. In my opinion, they were more of a fatigue inducing situation than anything else. The listener seldom got any real training, they were not exposed to the forced choice scenario until it was time to 'perform', and they were not really trained in terms of what kinds of things to listen for, what kinds of things to hone in on, etc.

The music was typically left to play on, and this is a huge error in procedure. In essence, the listener was never comparing the same signal on both DUTs at any given time, in fact, the same signal was NEVER compared, ONLY a different signal was ever compared. This is such a big problem with the procedure, that such listening tests could be summarily dismissed as an invalid attempt based on this alone.

In terms of listening fatigue, the listener was encouraged to switch back and forth as often and as much as they desired, and this often lead inexperienced and untrained listeners to switch back and forth a huge number of times, all the while not really focusing in on the musical presentation that much. Again, with the music playing on, it would be very hard to try and draw any sort of valid choice, and just as hard to hear what the two units were doing even when you knew which one was which.

This combined with the typically open ended initial sighted portion, and the relatively large number of trials, each of which might include dozens or even hundreds of times that the listener switched back and forth betwen the various units, in my opinion was the cause f a lot of listener fatigue, and therefore was also a very significant factor in these kinds of tests coming up with null results.

Then there was the issue of the switchbox itself, and the extra cables, often of a very poor overall quality level. The relays inside the ABX boxes were of various types over the years, the early ones were mercury wetted reed-relays, the later ones were supposedly rheuthenium plated relay contacts. It has been argued that the switchbox was a source of significant degradation of the listening test resolving power, due to the extra cables and contacts involved. The signal was exposed to magnetic fields inside the relay, and had to travel through a lot of extra wiring and contacts compared to a normal direct real-world connection.

Defenders claimed that the ABX switchbox had passed two tests that assured it was transparent, aside from the usual objective measurement standards of THD, noise and the like:
One, it had been tested using yet another ABX switchbox, and the results had turned up as a null.
Two, J. Gordon Holt, the golden-ears of Stereophile fame, was said to have found it to be 'inaudible' during one of his listening sessions once long ago.

Well, I hope that I don't have to explain the fallacy involved with the first assertion, and the second one is ironic, as one of the very things that the ABX folks were against, was the acceptance of any pronouncements from golden-eared reviewers using sighted listening to review audio products. I think it incredible that they wanted to dismiss and discount all the other reviewers, and Mr. Holt as well when he was reviewing audio equipment, but it was OK to accept his pronouncement on THEIR unit as being transparent when using the same methods. Even so, it is a good idea to note that this occurred back in the 80's, so who knows what one would hear using modern high performance audio gear?

Finally, the confidence level chosen, as well as the particular number of trials, created a very high 'bar' to hurtle, the listener had to be really hearing definite things, and would not have been able to easily discern more subtle things to the requirements chosen.

Despite all of this, certain folks try to cite these old DBT tests as definitive evidence of no sonic differences for audio cables, CDPs, power amps, etc. Not only are the previous problems cited good reasons not to do this, even if none of the problems had existed, and all the items objected to been corrected, there would still be a fundamental problem with doing so.

This fundamental problem is the equating of a null result, that is, a listening test result that simply failed to reach the previously defined criteria for a statistically significant result, as a negative result.

If you have a controlled listening test, and it fails to reach the defined level of statistical confidence, then the result is often called a null result, or "accepting the null hypothesis". However, this kind of result really and truly has no other meaning. You can not legitimately equate a null result to a negative.

Some folks have tried to argue that the equating of a null with a negative is legitimate, and even cited a lone book as a reference. However, the vast majority of statistics books, professors, and accepted authorities still maintain that doing so is just not correct.

The primary reason for not doing so was touched on earlier, you can not know how sensitive the listening test set-up is, unless you have performed a control experiment to determine this. Without such a control, a test that has determined how sensitive both the listening test set-up, and the listening subjects are to very subtle sound issues, you can not have any chance of knowing that the listening test was even inherently capable of discerning what was being tested for!

In the ABX style listening tests, the comments by listeners in the sighted portion that they are indeed hearing what they expected to hear is often cited as a sufficient provision of this test sensitivity control information.
However, this is NOT a scientific way to achieve the determination of this control condition. It is another example of the answer begging the question. Just as you can not use the test to test the test, you can not use a sighted portion to verify the performance of the forced choice portion. This is yet another example of the incorrect reasoning used to justify these kinds of listening tests, and how valid they are supposed to be.

Part 2 will cover the inherent problems and flaws with DBT listening tests, even when done impeccably. Part 3 will cover alternate methods and include some comments on doing your own DBT's.


Jon Risch


DBTs, ABX and the Meaning of Life? Part 2

This is as much a continuation of Part 1 as it is a separate Part 2.

I covered some of the problems encountered in the real world amateur DBTs that have been conducted, but what about a well done DBT, how much resolving power can a good DBT reach?

There are several issues here that need to be addressed.

Obviously, the playback system needs to be capable of revealing any sonic differences that are being investigated. This does not necessarily mean that one should use the most expensive, or the most "SOTA" system available. Often, a system the listener/s are intimately familiar with can be more useful than one of theoretically greater resolving power. But even the familiar system needs to be inherently capable of revealing the subtleties of what is being searched for.

In lieu of achieving positive listening test results for the audio component being investigated, one can use other means to determine what the listening system CAN actually detect.
How would this be done? Well, we can use known thresholds of detection for such repeatable and controllable items such as THD, or amplitude changes.

The strong suite of an ABX style test is detecting amplitude changes, and as reported at the ABX website, they were able to detect down to a 0.3 dB overall level change. If I am remembering correctly, jj has posted that he believes that with a really good test, one could get down to detecting a level of about 0.1 dB overall level change.

Or, you could determine what the sensitivity of the listening test set-up and the listeners was, to say, simple harmonic distortion. For instance, it is not too difficult to detect 2% 3rd harmonic distortion of a 1 kHz tone. The threshold is around 0.5% to 0.3%, depending on whose data you believe. So if a particular test came up empty during the audio component testing, and was determined to be unable to detect 1% 3rd HD at 1 kHz, and could only detect 0.5 dB overall level change, then there is some indication of the level of sensitivity of that particular test, and maybe it just wasn't sensitive enough to detect the more subtle aspects of music reproduction. The null results for the audio components may not have had very much meaning.

Note that if a test has been well designed and executed, and has none of the potential flaws or traps one can get into, so as to generate trivially false positives, then consistently achieving statistically significant positives for the audio component test is in and of itself, a form of validation of the sensitivity of the test. False positives can result from something as innocent as a very slight residual hum present only on one of the DUTs, and so, when asked to ID the X choice, the presence or absence of the hum would be a dead giveaway. Obviously, these kinds of problems would have to be addressed fully in order to conduct a valid test.

Failure to properly level match could also create such a problem, and there are other similar kinds of problems that could crop up, and need to be addressed.

All of the above concerns can be dealt with and the problems eliminated, but there is a more fundamental problem with many listening tests. A forced choice type of listening test also has a dichotomy between a casual listening mode and an analytical listening mode.

During an ABX style test, where the listeners were exposed to sighted listening of the components, and then were switched to blind listening, this would have involved a very casual listening atmosphere initially. So when they listen initially, they are listening casually, and without any special focus, perhaps believing they are indeed hearing the nominal differences between components, whether they actually are or not. The fact that they think they hear such differences, or that they claim to hear them, is often used as if it was some sort of major proof that they were listening attentively, when the exact opposite is probably true.

As I have conducted my own such tests, I am familiar with the foibles and problems of such tests. People will NOT listen attentively or focus on the sounds within the music unless you force them to, or direct their attention specifically to do so. Especially people without any experience or training in participating in such types of testing.

Now, when the listening session went from sighted to blind, and the listeners were asked to make a forced choice (identification of X is a forced choice, unless you allow for the option of the listener declaring that he could not reliably detect any differences, or that what he heard was identical. These are two different things BTW), the listening mode switched from casual listening to an analytical
mode. In order to decide and MAKE A DECISION that X is A, or X is B, or even that A and B are the same/can not tell, one must become analytical.

Instead of listening casually and in what some would refer to as an emotional listening mode (left brain activity), the listener is now asked to listen in analytical mode (right brain activity). The situation has changed, the listening mode is no longer the same, and all the problems and pressures of forced choices rear their ugly head.

Oh, you can say that the listener can take all the time he wants, or that there is no pressure, etc. These are all empty claims if you haven't been in the hot seat.
 

sxr7171

Diamond Member
Jun 21, 2002
5,079
40
91
Continued:

That is why I propose a more focused and attentive method of listening in my AES paper, I am trying to place the listener into an analytical mode for as much of the listening as possible, and then staying there to make the forced choice. Much more consistent results are then obtained.

Long term listening is critical to the more subtle ear/brain training processes, and allow us to learn to hear the more subtle aspects even once we enter the analytical listening mode. Without the casual listening practice and exposure, we would find it much more difficult to hear such things analytically, in most cases, it becomes too difficult. Training with such aids as I mention will help overcome some of this, but not all of it.

Note that this is NOT meant to be an end all ultimate way to do these kinds of tests. It is presented merely as a starting point for a more controlled method than the typical A vs. B audiophile type listening tests, and you can make it as involved, or as casual as you want, with the understanding that it will have whatever inherent limitations there are for that level of involvement.

Jon Risch





 

dman

Diamond Member
Nov 2, 1999
9,110
0
76
Honestly, I don't doubt that there may be some differences between cables, but, imo, an audiophile isn't going to get the cheap(er) stuff and be happy with it for long. If they can afford to buy the more expensive stuff they will. It's all about the marketing and if something is cheaper then they are loosing in the marketing game.
 
Nov 25, 2002
27
0
0
WOOOHOO - YOU WIN THE LONGEST POST OF THE MONTH PRIZE!!! WOOHOOOO!!!!

please just provide a link (if one exists), since you are filling ATs db with data not directly linked to the topic.

 

Sanjoelo

Senior member
Apr 4, 2001
809
0
0
Originally posted by: LegendKiller
Have YOU people ever listened to a good audio setup that has crappy cables and then switched to good ones?


What this basically comes down to, is there are haves and have-nots.

And pompous tools. Don't forget the pompous tools..

 

etee

Member
May 31, 2001
169
0
0
excellent post jbtech. it was serious and hilarious at the same time. get some fresh air ppl.
 

slacker2

Member
May 8, 2000
93
0
0
Well,

I admit I am firmly in the "non-believer" camp, simply because high-end audio is a market that's a gold mine for charlatans and snake oil dealers of every kind. However, one of the things that made me a non-believer was reading discussions between audiophiles and engineers on various forums - in the end audiophiles would invariably run out of rational arguments.

And heck, who is happier? A guy who buys a $100 stereo at Best Buy and enjoys the latest rap CD, or an audiophile who buys...oh, right, no CDs, gotta be lamps and vinyl. And not just any vinyl, but the one that was recorded at a certain studio and manufactured by a certain company in a certain year and month and when the phases of the moon were just right. Then the turntable and the pre-pre-pre-amp and the pre-preamp and the preamp and the amp and the post-amp and the Tice Clock have to be placed on a certain rack and of course gotta have those super-duper cables and super-duper power cords and interconnects and the speakers each have to be placed on a maple platform with spiked feet with a Shakti Stone on top. And you know what? It's never enough and it's a never-ending cycle of buying new equipment and gadgets. I dunno, to me this looks like a waste...